The emotion machine pdf free download






















Love, n. That disposition or state of feeling with regard to a person which arising from recognition of attractive qualities, from instincts of natural relationship, or from sympathy manifests itself in solicitude for the welfare of the object, and usually also in delight in his or her presence and desire for his or her approval; warm affection, attachment.

Yet even this larger conception of Love is still too narrow to cover enough, because Love is a kind of suitcase-like word, which includes other kinds of attachments like these:. We also apply that same word Love to our involvements with objects, feelings, ideas, and beliefs—and not only to ones that are sudden and brief, but also to bonds that increase through the years. Why do we pack such dissimilar things into those single suitcase-words?

Thus we use the word Anger to abbreviate a diverse collection of mental states, some of which change our ways to perceive, so that innocent gestures get turned into threats—and thus make us more inclined to attack. Fear also affects the ways we react but makes us retreat from dangerous things as well as from some that might please us too much. Returning to the meanings of Love, one thing seems common to all those conditions: each leads us to think in different ways:. This book is mainly filled with ideas about what could happen inside our brains to cause such great changes in how we think.

In short, we all need better ideas about the ways in which we think. But whenever we start to think about that, we encounter yet more mysteries. Now, everyone knows how Anger feels—or Pleasure, Sorrow, Joy, and Grief—yet we still know almost nothing about how those processes actually work.

As Alexander Pope asks in his Essay on Man, are these things that we can hope to understand? How did we manage to find out so much about atoms and oceans and planets and stars—yet so little about the mechanics of minds? Thus, Newton discovered just three simple laws that described the motions of all sorts of objects; Maxwell uncovered just four more laws that explained all electromagnetic events; then Einstein reduced all those and more into yet smaller formulas.

Then, why did the sciences of the mind make less progress in those same three centuries? I suspect that this was largely because most psychologists mimicked those physicists, by looking for equally compact solutions to questions about mental processes. However, that strategy never found small sets of laws that accounted for, in substantial detail, any large realms of human thought. So this book will embark on the opposite quest: to find more complex ways to depict mental events that seem simple at first!

This policy may seem absurd to scientists who have been trained to believe such statements as, One should never adopt hypotheses that make more assumptions than one needs. But it is worse to do the opposite—as when we use psychology words that mainly hide what they try to describe. For, look at suppresses your questions about the systems that choose how you move your eyes.

Then, object diverts you from asking how your visual systems partition a scene into various patches of color and texture—and then assign them to different things. It is the same for most of the commonsense words we use when we try to describe the events in minds—as when one makes a statement like, I think I understood what you said. Perhaps the most extreme examples of this are when we use words like you and me, because we all grow up with this fairy tale:. We each are constantly being controlled by powerful creatures inside our minds who do our feeling and thinking for us, and make our important decisions for us.

We call these our Selves or Identities —and we believe that they always remain the same, no matter how we may otherwise change. This Single-Self concept serves us well in our everyday social affairs. But it hinders our efforts to think about what minds are and how they work—because, when we ask about what Selves actually do, we get the same answer to every such question:. Your Self sees the world by using your senses.

Then it stores what it learns in your memory. It originates all your desires and goals—and then solves all your problems for you, by exploiting your intelligence. Here are a few kinds of reasons why a mind might entertain such a fiction:.

Child psychologist: As a child, you learned to distinguish among some persons in your environment. Later, you somehow came to conclude that you are such a person, too—but at the same time, you may have assumed that there is a person inside of you.

Practical person: That image makes us efficient, whereas better ideas might slow us down. It would take too long for our hardworking minds to understand everything all the time. However, although the Single-Self concept has practical uses, it does not help us to understand ourselves—because it does not provide us with smaller parts we could use to build theories of what we are.

When you think of yourself as a single thing, this gives you no clues about issues like these:. Whenever we wonder about our minds, the simpler are the questions we ask, the harder it seems to find answers to them. When asked about a complex physical task like, How could a person build a house, you might answer almost instantly, Make a foundation and then build walls and a roof.

However, we find it much harder to think of what to say about seemingly simpler questions like these:. Of course, those questions are not really simple at all.

To see an object or speak a word involves hundreds of different parts of your brain, each of which does some quite difficult jobs. Whenever you think about your Self, you are switching among a huge network of models, each of which tries to represent some particular aspects of your mind—to answer some questions about yourself. William James If one should seek to name each particular one of them of which the human heart is the seat, each race of men having found names for some shade of feeling which other races have left undiscriminated…all sorts of groupings would be possible, according as we chose this character or that as a basis.

The only question would be, does this grouping or that suit our purpose best? Sometimes a person gets into a state where everything seems to be cheerful and bright—although nothing outside has actually changed.

Other times everything pleases you less: the entire world seems dreary and dark, and your friends complain that you seem depressed. Why do we have such states of mind—or moods, or feelings, or dispositions—and what causes all their strange effects? Here are some of the phrases we find when dictionaries define emotion.

What is subjective supposed to mean, and what could a conscious affection be? In what ways do those parts of consciousness become involved with what we call feelings? Must every emotion involve a disturbance? Why do so many such questions arise when we try to define what emotion means? Here are a few of the hundreds of terms that we use to refer to our mental conditions:. Whenever you change your mental state, you might try to use those emotion-words to try to describe your new condition—but usually each such word or phrase refers to too wide a range of states.

Many researchers have spent their lives at classifying our states of mind, by arranging terms like feelings, dispositions, tempers, and moods into orderly charts or diagrams—but should we call Anguish a feeling or a mood? Is Sorrow a type of disposition? No one can settle the use of such terms because different traditions make different distinctions, and different people have different ideas about how to describe their various states of mind.

How many readers can claim to know precisely how each of the following feelings feels? In everyday life, we expect our friends to know what we mean by Pleasure or Fear —but I suspect that attempting to make our old words more precise has hindered more than helped us to make theories about how human minds work.

So this book will take a different approach, by thinking of each mental condition as based on the use of many small processes. Charles Darwin Infants, when suffering even slight pain, moderate hunger, or discomfort, utter violent and prolonged screams.

Whilst thus screaming their eyes are firmly closed, so that the skin round them is wrinkled, and the forehead contracted into a frown. The mouth is widely opened with the lips retracted in a peculiar manner, which causes it to assume a squarish form; the gums or teeth being more or less exposed.

One moment your baby seems perfectly well, but then come some restless motions of limbs. Next you see a few catches of breath, and then suddenly the air fills with screams. Is baby hungry, sleepy, or wet? Whatever the trouble may turn out to be, those cries compel you to find some way to help—and once you find the remedy, things quickly return to normal.

In the meantime though, you, too, feel distressed. Soon after birth you can usually sense that a particular baby reacts more quickly than others, or seems more patient or irritable, or even more inquisitive. Some of those traits may change with time, but others persist throughout life. Nevertheless, we still need to ask, What could make an infant so suddenly switch, between one moment and the next, from contentment or calmness to anger or rage?

You could start by making a list of goals that your animal-robot needs to achieve. It may need to find parts with which to repair itself. It may need defenses against attacks. Perhaps it should regulate its temperature. It may even need ways to attract helpful friends. Then once you have assembled that list, you could tell your engineers to meet each of those needs by building a separate instinct-machine —and then to package them all into a single body-box.

What goes inside each instinct-machine? Each of them needs three kinds of resources: some ways to recognize situations, some knowledge about how to react to these, and some muscles or motors to execute actions. What goes inside that knowledge box? Then all we need is a catalog of simple, two-part "If Do" rules—where each If describes one of those situations, and each Do describes an action to take.

Every infant animal is born with many If Do rules like these. For example, each human infant is born with ways to maintain its body temperature: when too hot, it can pant, sweat, stretch out, or vasodilate; when too cold, it can shiver, retract its limbs, or vasoconstrict—or metabolize to produce more heat.

Then later in life, we learn to use actions that change the external world. It would be naive to try to describe a mind as nothing more than bundles of If Do rules. This sketch shows only a part of the structure that Tinbergen proposed to explain how a certain fish behaves. Of course, it would need much more than this to support the higher levels of human thought. The rest of this book will describe some ideas about the structures inside our human minds.

Open navigation menu. Close suggestions Search Search. User Settings. Skip carousel. Carousel Previous. Carousel Next. What is Scribd? Cancel anytime. Start your free 30 days Read preview. Released: Nov 13, ISBN: Format: Book. In this mind-expanding book, scientific pioneer Marvin Minsky continues his groundbreaking research, offering a fascinating new model for how our minds work.

He argues persuasively that emotions, intuitions, and feelings are not distinct things, but different ways of thinking. By examining these different forms of mind activity, Minsky says, we can explain why our thought sometimes takes the form of carefully reasoned analysis and at other times turns to emotion. He shows how our minds progress from simple, instinctive kinds of thought to more complex forms, such as consciousness or self-awareness.

And he argues that because we tend to see our thinking as fragmented, we fail to appreciate what powerful thinkers we really are. Indeed, says Minsky, if thinking can be understood as the step-by-step process that it is, then we can build machines -- artificial intelligences -- that not only can assist with our thinking by thinking as we do but have the potential to be as conscious as we are.

Eloquently written, The Emotion Machine is an intriguing look into a future where more powerful artificial intelligences await. About the author. Read more. What Is Your Dangerous Idea? Are Numbers Real? Related Podcast Episodes. Artificial Intelligence: Melvyn Bragg investigates artificial intelligence; can a computer imitate the human mind?

A life-long athlete and recently graduated college Erin Podcast 11 min listen. Joe Dispenza - Becoming Supernatural: How Common People Are Doing the Uncommon: Rewire your brain by taking a journey in brain evolution using scientifically proven neurophysiological principles.

Joe Dispenza not only teaches people the science of change, he shows the results through rigorous scientific testing including Marvin Minsky: Forward Thinker: Who was Marvin Minsky and what was his influence on the field of artificial intelligence research? We look back on the life and career of this forward thinker.

As a former criminal carrying the baggage of a tough upbringing, he created a process to overhaul Dan Siegel by The Psychology Podcast 43 min listen. My team and I have put together some of the very best ideas shared on my show which I believe, if you start impleme AI brings us to the age of information and technology, whereas Victorian literature invites us to the world of lengthy novels, to the world of the written word.

In sobriety, it's possible to become more in tune with the inner workings of our minds. By stopping the intake of a numbing substance, our minds and bodies become more sensitive, and Caroline Leaf: Manage your mind… Control your mess!

We all have a mess in our brains. How we are able to manage that mess is the difference between a healthy mind and body and a mind and body riddled with anxiety and depression. And there is no one better on the Caroline Leaf on Health Theory: Dr. And that potential looks to be almost limitless. Talking Neuroscience with Commander Divine: In this solo episode, Commander Divine continues with his reading of the updated version of his Unbeatable Mind book.

Today, he discusses the different parts of the brain and different ways of thinking. The question whether or not film produces genuine emotion is answered by comparing affect in the viewer with emotion in the real world experienced by persons witnessing events that have personal significance to them. Current understanding of the psychology of emotions provides the basis for identifying critic.

Heart of the Machine explores the next giant step in the relationship between humans and technology: the ability of computers to recognize, respond to, and even replicate emotions. Computers have long been integral to our lives, and their advances continue at an exponential rate. Many believe that artificial intelligence equal or superior to human intelligence will happen in the not-too-distance future; some even think machine consciousness will follow.

Futurist Richard Yonck argues that emotion, the first, most basic, and most natural form of communication, is at the heart of how we will soon work with and use computers. Instilling emotions into computers is the next leap in our centuries-old obsession with creating machines that replicate humans. But for every benefit this progress may bring to our lives, there is a possible pitfall.

Emotion recognition could lead to advanced surveillance, and the same technology that can manipulate our feelings could become a method of mass control. And, as shown in movies like Her and Ex Machina, our society already holds a deep-seated anxiety about what might happen if machines could actually feel and break free from our control. Heart of the Machine is an exploration of the new and inevitable ways in which mankind and technology will interact.

Beef : he said do you ever feel what you'd call a "negative sensation? The thought controls the body through emotions. The body affects the thought through emotions. Through this mec- nism, the thought allows the agent to behave intelligently in the complex world filled with a huge amount of dynamic information.

Recent findings in brain science suggest that mirror neurons map visual signals into motor signals for the body. In particular, it might play a significant role in invoking empathy in a social situation.

It may not be hard to think about what might happen to emotion-less machines. The emotion-less machines may not be able to accumulate experiences to avoid serious failures. Share This Paper. Background Citations. Methods Citations. Results Citations. Supplemental Video.

Computer Science. Citation Type. Has PDF. Publication Type. More Filters. For the last decade, Buddhists have engaged in dialog with the cognitive sciences about the nature of consciousness and the self Wallace This dialog has made clear that Buddhist psychology … Expand.

Highly Influenced. View 4 excerpts, cites background. Text comprehension and the computational mind-agencies. View 15 excerpts, cites background and methods. This chapter explores how our brains and minds are split. The two brain hemispheres balance details and wholes, and our minds balance emotions and thinking.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000